1/2018
vol. 12
Factors determining the level of health behaviours amongst patients and healthy individuals
Katarzyna Cierzniakowska
,
Pielęgniarstwo Chirurgiczne i Angiologiczne 2018; 1: 20-26
Online publish date: 2018/04/10
Get citation
Introduction
The significant roles of environment and lifestyle in determining human health have been known for a long time. These two areas overlap and complete each other because lifestyle sometimes determines the degree of exposure to harmful environmental factors. The health awareness of a community is very important from the viewpoint of health prevention [1]. By having sufficient knowledge in regard to risk factors for various diseases, individuals with high health awareness may avoid these factors whenever possible or reduce their exposure to them. These individuals may also partake in regular prophylactic screening in order to detect potential diseases at early stages when the probability of recovery is markedly higher. These aforementioned activities, corresponding to the practical execution of health awareness, are known as health behaviours.
Health behaviours undoubtedly play important roles in determining the health status of communities, but the evaluation of these roles remains difficult. Proper health behaviours (or the lack of them) usually have long-term consequences, with the identification of respective cause-effect relationships having proven to be difficult. Research in this field is also complicated due to the complex characteristics of the activities involved in health behaviours.
Besides research limitations, the aforementioned problems cause many people to omit proper health behaviours despite having relatively high health awareness [2]. Usually patients begin to partake in health behaviours only after being diagnosed with a disease. Therefore, in many patients with chronic disease, primary prevention is in fact tertiary prevention. In the case of recurrent diseases, the term “secondarily primary prevention” seems to be the best description of patient activities. Due to the aforementioned research limitations and the lack of standardised research tools, observations of health behaviours in healthy and diseased individuals are sparse and based on case studies that have not been verified in large population studies.
The aim of this study was to evaluate the level of health behaviours in patients with different prognoses and in healthy participants of similar age based upon a standardised research tool – the Health Behaviour Inventory. We also determined the effects of demographic variables on the levels of health behaviours in these groups.
Material and methods
This study included 296 patients of the General, Gastroenterological, Colorectal, and Oncological Surgery Ward. This group included 99 patients hospitalised due to the primary surgical treatment of colorectal cancer (Group A) and 100 patients who were operated on due to gallstones (Group B). The control group (Group C) comprised 97 participants who were free from cancer or gallstones, and selected at random from visitors to the Clinic. The statistical characteristics of the study participants are summarised in Table 1.
Patients from Group A and B were subjected to a questionnaire survey during the pre-operative period, whereas participants in the control group completed the questionnaire during their visits to the Clinic. All participants were asked to complete the standardised questionnaire: Health Behaviour Inventory (HBI), kindly provided by the Workshop of Psychological Tests by the Polish Psychological Society in Warsaw. This questionnaire was developed to examine healthy and diseased adults. It contains 24 questions pertaining to the last 12 months and describes health behaviours in regard to four different aspects:
1) dietary habits (DH), 2) prophylactic behaviours (PB),
3) psychological attitude (PA), and 4) health practices (HP).
The answers to these questions were graded using a five-point scale where 1 corresponds to nearly never and 5 corresponds to nearly always. Based on this point system, the mean level of health behaviours was calculated for each of the four categories. The total HBI was calculated as the sum of all points gathered. Theoretically, the total HBI could range from 24 points (all answers graded 1) to 120 points (all answers graded 5). These values are presented and interpreted using the standard ten scale (women: low 24–77 pts, moderate 78–91 pts, high 92–120 pts; males: low 24–71 pts, moderate 72–86 pts, high 87–120 pts). Cronbach’s reliability index (alpha index) of HBI was calculated to be 0.85, ranging from 0.60 to 0.65 depending on the analysed category of health behaviours [3].
The three groups of subjects were compared in terms of mean HBI levels and mean levels of health behaviours in each of the four HBI categories. They were also compared in terms of HBI distribution using the standard ten scale. Additionally, associations between the sociodemographic characteristics of study participants and the values of HBI or its categories, or HBI distribution in the standard ten scale, were also analysed.
Continuous variables were presented as arithmetic means and their standard deviations (SD). Normal distribution was tested using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Arithmetic means between the A, B, and C groups were compared using ANOVA and the Tukey post-hoc test. Discrete variables were presented as number and percentage distributions. Their distributions amongst the groups were compared using the Pearson’s chi-square test. Calculations were performed using Statistica 7 (StatSoft®, Poland) software, with statistical significance defined as p ≤ 0.05.
Results
The studied groups did not differ significantly in terms of mean values of total HBI and its four categories (Figure 1, 2).
In colorectal cancer patients (Group A), significantly higher values of HBI were observed in women than in men (93.43 ±15.07 vs. 85.53 ±13.72, p = 0.007). Female patients of Group A had also significantly higher values of variables referring to dietary habits when compared to males (3.70 ±0.85 vs. 3.10 ±0.72, p < 0.001). No significant effects of gender on HBI values or its component levels were observed in the remaining groups. The only exception to this observation was in reference to the dietary habits of the control group, where the values were significantly higher in women than in men (3.60 ±0.64 vs. 3.21 ±0.94, p = 0.019). Male gallstone patients were characterised by significantly higher levels of dietary habits compared to men from other groups (3.50 ±0.71 vs. 3.10 ±0.72 and 3.21 ±0.94 in Groups A and C, respectively, p = 0.046). Women with colorectal cancer or gallstones predominated over female controls in terms of their psychological attitudes (4.14 ±0.63 and 3.98 ±0.56 in Groups A and B, respectively vs. 3.80 ±0.64 in Group C, p = 0.025).
In all groups, significantly higher values of HBI were observed in participants over 65 years of age. Also, mean values of the HP variable significantly increased with participant age in all studied groups. Additionally, PA values increased significantly in elderly subjects from Group A and C, along with the DH variable in controls (Table 2).
In colorectal cancer patients, significantly higher HBI values were observed in single subjects (94.70 ±12.98 vs. 87.14 ±15.04 in married individuals, p = 0.023). The single subjects from Group A were also characterised by significantly higher values of PA (4.39 ±0.48 vs. 3.86 ±0.63 in married individuals, p < 0.001) and HP variables (4.03 ±0.86 vs. 3.61 ±0.85 in married individuals, p = 0.031). In the case of the latter variable, significantly higher values were also noted amongst single subjects from the control group (3.95 ±0.62 vs. 3.49 ±0.78 in married individuals, p = 0.007). The mean value of PA in single colorectal cancer patients was significantly higher when compared to other groups (4.39 ±0.48 vs. 3.75 ±0.68 and 3.95 ±0.63 in Groups B and C, respectively, p < 0.001).
In all of the studied groups, significantly higher HBI levels were observed amongst non-working individuals. The lack of professional activity positively influenced all categories of HBI, with the exception of PB and DH in Group A and B, respectively (Table 3).
No significant differences were observed amongst the studied groups in terms of percentage distributions of participants who had high HBI values in the standard ten scale (above 92 pts and above 87 pts in women and men, respectively). No significant differences amongst the groups were noted after their stratification with grouping variables. Moreover, the grouping variables did not influence significantly the fraction of Group A, B, or C participants who had high HBI values. The only exception was employment status in colorectal cancer patients and in controls where high HBI values were observed significantly more frequently amongst non-workers (Group A: 53.42% in non-workers vs. 23.08% in workers, p = 0.008; Group C: 50.82% in non-workers vs. 22.22% in workers, p = 0.006).
Discussion
This study revealed no significant differences amongst colorectal and gallstone patients and healthy controls in terms of the overall health behaviour levels and values of particular HBI components. However, significant differences in health behaviour levels were observed following participant stratification in regard to some sociodemographic features.
We confirmed the significant effect of gender on health behaviour levels amongst colorectal cancer patients. In this group, women were characterised by higher values of HBI and its DH component when compared to males. Better dietary habits were also observed in women from the control group. According to the HBI, proper dietary habits include avoidance of chemically preserved foods, eating cereals and high quantities of fruits and vegetables, along with a reduced consumption of animal fats and sugars as well as salt and salted food products [3]. In males, these aforementioned health behaviours were enforced by participants only in cases where an acute diet-dependent condition – gallstones – was diagnosed. This was probably the reason why gallstone patients were the only group in which gender had no significant influence on the level of dietary habits.
No significant gender-related differences were noted in terms of other HBI components in our population. The lack of gender effects on values of HBI and its components was also observed by Kozieł et al. [4] in their study of elderly people. However, this finding does not seem to be universally true because Slusarska and Nowicki [5] revealed that mean HBI values in professionally active women are higher than in working males.
In a study of women after mastectomy, published by Andruszkiewicz and Ozminska [6], HBI levels were slightly higher than 90 points and therefore similar as in our subgroup of female colorectal cancer patients. In our study, however, women with cancer were characterised by better psychological attitudes than healthy controls. Patients after mastectomy, in turn, did not predominate over the healthy controls in terms of psychological attitudes but had higher values of prophylactic behaviours and variables of health practices [6].
Participant age was another factor that significantly influenced levels of health behaviours in this study. Subjects aged 65 years or older took care of their health better than younger participants, particularly in terms of health practices. According to the HBI, these health practices included proper management of leisure time, avoidance of working too much, control of body mass, and the cessation of smoking. Taking these aforementioned components into account, the increase in the amount of spare time due to retirement (along with proven age-related health deficits) seems to be the most probable reason for higher HP values observed amongst the elderly participants of this study. This hypothesis seems to be at least partly confirmed by higher values of health practice variables observed amongst non-working or single individuals. Lower values of HBI amongst professionally active individuals were also described by Slusarska and Nowicki [5].
Interestingly, the HBI levels of elderly people participating in this study were higher than in seniors participating in the Third Age University projects in Kielce (Eastern Poland), who were studied by Kozieł et al. [4]. Plausibly, this variability may be related to differences in geographic locations. According to the literature, this factor – even if minute – may constitute an important determinant of health behaviour levels [7].
Published evidence suggests that mean HBI values in adolescents do not exceed 80 points [8]. Alarmingly, low levels of health behaviours were also observed amongst nursing students, i.e. in the group that theoretically should be prepared for the role of health educators [9]. Meanwhile, prophylactic behaviours constituted the lowest scoring HBI component amongst Polish adolescents [8]. In contrast, a Turkish study revealed a high level of knowledge pertaining to cancer prevention amongst students from local medical universities [10].
The relatively low level of prophylactic behaviours is also an alarming finding amongst the participants of this study. According to the HBI, prophylactic behaviours include the avoidance of catching a cold, easy access to the telephone numbers of medical emergency services, following physicians’ prescriptions and recommendations, the regular performance of medical tests, and seeking information on disease aetiology and prevention [3]. According to the literature, health information is more frequently sought by elderly people, women, and individuals with higher levels of education [11]. Surprisingly, our study did not confirm the effects of gender or age on levels of health behaviours. Other evidence pointing to the low health awareness of Polish women is represented by the fact that only 25% of women participate in free cervical cancer screenings, despite receiving individual invitations [12].
In this study, the HBI component pertaining to psychological attitudes was scored highest out of all analysed categories of health behaviours. The PA component included: serious attitudes towards the suggestions of people who worry about the participant’s health, the avoidance of depressing situations along with strong emotions, stress, tension, anger, anxiety, and depression, having friends and structure in life, and positive thinking [3]. Noticeably, when compared to other HBI categories, these aforementioned components of psychological attitude are hard to measure and verify, being the potential explanation for the higher values recorded in the PA component as declared by study participants. Similar conclusions originate from other studies using HBI [5]. However, one should not underestimate the role of stress in determining health behaviours because it was revealed to be one of the factors reducing levels of prophylactic behaviours [13]. Emotions play a very particular role during the interpretation of an individual’s cancer risk [14], and a study of males from a prostate cancer risk group revealed stress to be one of the principle factors responsible for delays in performing prophylactic screening for this malignancy [15].
The results of this study, along with the relevant literature review, suggest that modifications of health behaviours are needed in all age groups of Polish citizens [16, 17, 18]. The cost of this type of healthcare and low health awareness constitute the main barriers limiting the spread of health behaviours within communities [1]. Moreover, one of the crucial manifestations of health behaviours, namely participation in prophylactic screening, is influenced by personal and social factors [19]. Additionally, a focus group study revealed that nearly half of the participants do not participate in prophylactic screening despite sufficient knowledge regarding its benefits [2].
A literature review indicates that physicians should play the main role in health education and proper health behaviour implementation [20]. However, this is not always possible; in Polish conditions, the educational mission of physicians is hindered by the inefficiency of the public healthcare system and a continuous decrease in the authority of doctors. Therefore, the educational role of physicians should be (and sometimes is) taken over by mass media [21]. However, it is of crucial importance to deliver health information that is clear, understandable, and adjusted to the level of perception of the target group [20, 22–24]. The modern mass media may provide an opportunity for individualised remote modification of public health behaviours. According to an Australian study, dietary habits and physical activity in patients with type 2 diabetes or hypertension could be efficiently modified by means of telephone-delivered intervention [25]. One should also never forget the health education of people who participate in prophylactic screening [26]. Personal and familial history of disease, particularly in terms of malignant neoplasms, is an important factor possibly influencing health behaviours of individuals [27]. An example of an efficient, low-cost intervention utilising oncologic pedigrees as the basis for the health education of cancer patient families is the prophylactic program ongoing in Lower Silesia – the southwestern region of Poland, which is characterised by some of the highest incidence rates of malignant neoplasms [28, 29].
Conclusions
In conclusion, this study confirmed that one can find relatively low levels of health behaviours in Polish society, and highlighted potential sociodemographic features that should be considered during the modification of this unfavourable situation.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Jimenez EA, Xie Y, Goldsteen K, et al. Promoting knowledge of cancer prevention and screening in an underserved Hispanic women population: a culturally sensitive education program. Health Promot Pract 2011; 12: 689-695.
2. Carter VL, Dawkins NL, Howard B. Healthy Lifestyle: a community-based cancer awareness and prevention intervention program. J Health Care Poor Underserved 2010; 21: 107-118.
3. Juczynski Z. Narzędzia pomiaru w promocji i psychologii zdrowia; wyd. II. Polskie Towarzystwo Psychologiczne, Warszawa 2009.
4. Kozieł D, Kaczmarczyk M, Naszydłowska E, et al. The influence of studying at the Third Age University on elderly people’s health behaviour. Studia Med 2008; 12: 23-28.
5. Slusarska B, Nowicki G. Health behaviours in prophylaxis of cardiovascular diseases among occupationally active population. Probl Hig Epidemiol 2010; 91: 34-40.
6. Andruszkiewicz A, Basinska M. Health behaviors of nicotine addicts. Przegl Lek 2009; 66: 783-785.
7. Hughes EG, Peng X, Gleichman AJ, et al. Cellular and synaptic mechanisms of anti-NMDA receptor encephalitis. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 5866-5875.
8. Kozieł D, Naszydłowska E, Trawczynska M, et al. Youth’s healthy behavior as a direction for healthcare education. Zdrowie Publ 2003; 113: 280-284.
9. Lewko J, Politynska-Lewko B, Sierakowska M, et al. Health behaviours of nursing students. Annales UMCS, Sectio D Medicina 2005; LX(Suppl. XVI): 260-264.
10. Coban A, Kavlak O, Ozbasaran F. The evaluation of school of
health students’ knowledge and behavior of healthy life-styles of related to importance to prevent cancer. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev 2010; 11: 773-776.
11. Kelly B, Hornik R, Romantan A, et al. Cancer information scanning and seeking in the general population. J Health Commun 2010; 15: 734-753.
12. Spaczynski M, Karowicz-Bilinska A, Rokita W, et al. Attendance rate in the Polish Cervical Cancer Screening Program in the years 2007-2009. Ginekol Pol 2010; 81: 655-663.
13. O’Donnell S, Goldstein B, Dimatteo MR, et al. Adherence to mammography and colorectal cancer screening in women 50-80 years of age the role of psychological distress. Womens Health Issues 2010; 20: 343-349.
14. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Fagerlin A, Ubel PA. Risky feelings: why a 6% risk of cancer does not always feel like 6%. Patient Educ Couns 2010; 81 Suppl: S87-93.
15. Kleier JA. Fear of and susceptibility to prostate cancer as predictors of prostate cancer screening among Haitian-American men. Urol Nurs 2010; 30: 179-188.
16. Cierzniakowska K, Westphal B, Szewczyk MT i wsp. Health behaviour in group of patients with carotid arteries narrowing. Pielęg Chir Angiol 2011; 4: 211-217.
17. Michalak S, Cierzniakowska K, Banaszkiewicz Z i wsp. Evaluation of patients adjustment to living with stoma. Pielęg Chir Angiol 2008; 3: 91-98.
18. Cierzniakowska K, Szewczyk MT, Kozłowska E i wsp. Nursing care for perioperative diabetic patients in the aspect of postoperative complications. Pielęg Chirur Angiol 2017; 11: 94-100.
19. Yang RJ, Huang LH, Hsieh YS, et al. Motivations and reasons for women attending a breast self-examination training program: A qualitative study. BMC Womens Health 2010; 10: 23.
20. Jepson RG, Harris FM, Platt S, et al. The effectiveness of interventions to change six health behaviours: a review of reviews. BMC Public Health 2010; 10: 538.
21. Wakefield MA, Loken B, Hornik RC. Use of mass media campaigns to change health behaviour. Lancet 2010; 376: 1261-1271.
22. Clark BK, Sugiyama T, Healy GN, et al. Socio-demographic correlates of prolonged television viewing time in Australian men and women: the AusDiab study. J Phys Act Health 2010; 7: 595-601.
23. Mazor KM, Calvi J, Cowan R, et al. Media messages about cancer: what do people understand? J Health Commun 2010; 15 Suppl 2: 126-145.
24. Smith SK, Trevena L, Simpson JM, et al. A decision aid to support informed choices about bowel cancer screening among adults with low education: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 2010; 34: c5370.
25. Eakin E, Reeves M, Winkler E, et al. Maintenance of physical activity and dietary change following a telephone-delivered intervention. Health Psychol 2010; 29: 566-573.
26. van der Aalst CM, van Klaveren RJ, de Koning HJ. Does participation to screening unintentionally influence lifestyle behaviour and thus lifestyle-related morbidity? Best Pract Res Clin Gastroenterol 2010; 24: 465-478.
27. Salz T, Gottlieb SL, Smith JS, et al. The association between cervical abnormalities and attitudes toward cervical cancer prevention. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 2010; 19: 2011-2016.
28. Bebenek M, Błaszczyk J, Antczak A. Significance of oncological pedigree for the identification of familial predisposition to cancer. Onkologia Pol 2006; 9: 41-46.
29. Bebenek M., Rutkowska A, Błaszczyk J. Prevalence of hereditary non-polyposis colorectal cancer among Lower Silesians. Gastroenterol Pol 2006; 13: 457-461.
Copyright: © 2018 Termedia Sp. z o. o. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), allowing third parties to copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format and to remix, transform, and build upon the material, provided the original work is properly cited and states its license.
|
|